Literacy boosts language
6 RepliesWashington DC’s amazing Planet Word Museum has a great YouTube channel which includes a lecture called Eyes on Reading: Dr. Stanislas Dehaene with Emily Hanford about how learning to read changes the brain. I’ve been thinking a lot lately about the many kids I’ve known with whose language test scores vastly improved after they learnt to read and write, so I sat up and paid close attention when Dr. Dehaene said (starting at 16.15 on the video clock):
“…because you’ve learned to read, you are processing spoken language better. You are hearing the phonemes better and you are able to do phonological awareness tasks, you’re able to move phonemes around in your mind, you’re able to play with phonemes, and we found that there is almost a doubling of the brain activity for spoken language as a function of how good a reader you are, as a function of reading score. So that’s a huge transformation. We don’t fully know at the cellular level what it means, but there is a huge enhancement of responses in this area. We also found that the connection between these systems, if we look just at the anatomy of the connections of the brain with diffusion imaging, was reinforced “.
Matthew Effects and Spiral Causality
We’ve known since Keith Stanovich’s classic 1986 Matthew Effects article that reading boosts both language and cognitive skills. He explains this here:
Conversely, failing to learn to read has a negative impact on language and cognitive skills, and other negative knock-on effects, as Stanovich wrote:
“Slow reading acquisition has cognitive, behavioral, and motivational consequences that slow the development of other cognitive skills and inhibit performance on many academic tasks. In short, as reading develops, other cognitive processes linked to it track the level of reading skill. Knowledge bases that are in reciprocal relationships with reading are also inhibited from further development. The longer this developmental sequence is allowed to continue, the more generalized the deficits will become, seeping into more and more areas of cognition and behavior. Or, to put it more simply – and more sadly – in the words of a tearful nine-year-old, already falling frustratingly behind his peers in reading progress, “Reading affects everything you do.” (p390)
In 2011, Dutch researchers Suzanne Mol and Adriana Bus published To Read or Not To Read: A Meta-Analysis of Print Exposure from Infancy to Early Adulthood, in which they found:
“Print exposure explains 12% of the variance in preschoolers’ and kindergartners’ oral language skills,13% in primary school, 19% in middle school, 30% in high school, and 34% at undergraduate and graduate level…Although these outcomes do not permit conclusions about causality, the pattern of findings as well as a qualitative review of longitudinal studies suggests that spiral causality is plausible.”
Being literate helps us remember spoken words
In 2022 I was surprised by radio reports of a new, nationalist Italian Prime Minister with an Irish-sounding name: Georgia Maloney. OK, Italy and Ireland are both in the EU, but, mi scusi? Google to the rescue. Giorgia Meloni. Facepalm. I’d applied the wrong orthographic skeletons – ideas we form about a word’s spelling from its pronunciation. They’re what we correct/clarify when we hear an unfamiliar name, ask ‘how do you spell that?’, then don’t write it down. We’re just trying to mentally store a high-quality lexical representation (sounds, word structure, meaning and spelling).
Generating orthographic skeletons is linked to better word recall (see last year’s delightfully-titled article Déjà-lu: When Orthographic Representations are Generated in the Absence of Orthography), just as oral vocabulary knowledge boosts retention of written words (see other non-paywalled articles about this here, here, here and here). Words are learnt more easily, even by dyslexic children, when presented in both spoken and printed form. Spellings help us symbolise and store sounds/word parts in memory. Other research on this Orthographic Facilitation process can be found here, here and here.
We all know strong oral language is the foundation of strong literacy skills, but language and literacy intertwine across the lifespan in a reciprocal way, so that becoming literate boosts oral language skills, especially vocabulary. US education, cognitive science and fairness blogger Natalie Wexler’s recent post “Want children to be good speakers? Teach them to write” argues that writing instruction also improves comprehension and the ability to use formal spoken language. All of us at Spelfabet are about to do SRSD training, as research suggests their approach to writing also has positive spinoffs for other aspects of communication and wellbeing.
New NDIS guidelines
Australia’s new National Disability Insurance Scheme (NDIS) guidelines say supports related to school education are not funded by the NDIS. Having been in local government, I find cost-shifting maddening, so I agree that every cent of NDIS funding should be spent helping people with disabilities to become more independent, find work, study and have greater choice and control over their lives.
Ideally, schools should be able to teach all children with disabilities who can understand and use language to read and write. As well as this being great for the kids, it would save the NDIS a lot of money helping participants overcome consequences of poor literacy, like not being able to read signage, food labels, recipes, shopping lists, timetables, text messages etc., let alone study or find work.
Some children with disabilities have major difficulties with phonological processing, memory, speech, language, attention, perspective-taking, restricted interests, sensory overload, motor planning and/or anxiety, which interfere with learning to read and write. Even if they have high-quality classroom teaching and small group intervention in their first three years of schooling, they still fall behind their peers. Their disabilities require specialised, individualised intervention not always available in schools to learn to read and write, and thus improve their language and life prospects generally. The longer this intervention is postponed, the more difficult and expensive it becomes.
Alison Clarke
Speech Pathologist
Hi Alison,
I always love reading your newsletters, and this one is no exception.
Looking through the list of children’s disabilities, there are a few others that could be added, and which come from my own experience at school but which are relevant today with so much domestic violence being witnessed and/or given to young children. Being a year younger than others in the class added to worrying about a traumatic unloving home life, poverty, looking different, (I was very blonde), wearing secondhand shabby clothes, hair not done, walking two kilometers to catch the bus each morning, being constantly tired and hungry, often sick because of poor home hygiene, not dressed to suit the weather, and being scared of the boys in the class who would say things such as, ‘We’re going to bash you up this arvo’ were each a disadvantage. I turned off listening to the teacher as a result. I was too busy thinking about my problems. However, in the two weeks I went to a school in a bigger town to stay with my grandma while my mother had another baby, the teacher told her that I had changed from being the worst reader to the best reader. Even though I was never told what the teacher had said, those two weeks at the more friendly school changed my thinking that I was dumb because I was poor, and gave me the incentive to work hard to escape poverty.
My message is … children who daydream, find it hard to concentrate, have no friends and who rarely speak could be showing the signs of physical and emotional neglect, and/or could be too young for that class.
Hope this is of interest. Regards from a former teacher and now an author of teaching resources.
Hi Ellie, thanks for the nice feedback, sorry it got lost in a mountain of spam till now (I have spam filtering software but it seems not to be working RN) and I’m so sorry you had such a difficult childhood. How great you were able to overcome this and I’m sure it’s helped you create resources that are helpful to kids who struggle, whatever their reason. Yes, there are a lot of reasons beyond the ones that go with a diagnosis for kids having learning difficulties, and sometimes the school system can do its best to no avail, the problem lies elsewhere. Sigh.
Terribly disappointed in the new NDIS guidelines re: reading and writing. Ideally there’d be a full-time speech pathologist in every school – not to mention better methods of instruction. Until that’s a reality, reasonable overlap between education and health services should be permitted, no, encouraged…
Thank your for such an informative post Alison. I wish all educators knew about the connections between language modes and the importance and urgency of effective early reading and writing instruction.
Thanks for the nice feedback, Michelle. I guess we just need to keep working on overcoming the gaps in language knowledge that initial teacher education has bequeathed us, and hopefully won’t give us for too much longer. Heaps of educators are now educating their colleagues. Alison
Thank you Alison for summarising this information in a clear way as well as providing useful resources